neurodevelopmental.html

Neurodevelopmental status of HIV-exposed but uninfected children: A pilot study

P Springer, DCH (SA), FCPaed (SA)

B Laughton, DCH (SA), FCPaed (SA), MSc Med (Child Health Neurodevelopment)

Department of Paediatrics and Child Health, Tygerberg Children’s Hospital and Faculty of Health Sciences, Stellenbosch University, Western Cape

M Tomlinson, BA, BA Hons, MA (Clinical Psychology), PhD

Department of Psychology, Faculty of Health Sciences, Stellenbosch University, Western Cape

J Harvey, PhD (Mathematical Statistics)

Centre for Statistical Consultation, Stellenbosch University, Western Cape

M Esser, MMed (Paed)

Department of Pathology, Immunology Unit, Division of Medical Microbiology, National Health Laboratory Service, Stellenbosch University,
Tygerberg, Western Cape

Corresponding author: P Springer (springer@sun.ac.za)

Introduction. HIV affects children both directly and indirectly, with evidence of increased infectious mortality and morbidity in the HIV-exposed but uninfected (HEU) infant. There is little published research on neurodevelopmental outcome of HEU infants in Africa. Following the introduction of successful prevention of mother-to-child transmission programmes, it has become important to determine whether differences exist between HEU infants and infants born to HIV-negative mothers in order to guide current management policies of this rapidly growing group of infants.

Objectives. To compare the developmental outcome of infants exposed to HIV in utero who remained uninfected (HEU) with that of infants unexposed to HIV in utero (HUU).

Methodology. This was a prospective, blinded, hospital-based study. Infants aged between 17 and 19 months were assessed on the Griffiths Mental Developmental Scales (GMDS). Birth history, previous hospitalisation, maternal and infant characteristics, antiretroviral exposure, anthropometric measurements and abnormal clinical findings were documented.

Results. Of the original 55 infants enrolled at 2 weeks of age, 37 (17 HEU and 20 HUU) underwent neurological and developmental assessment. There were no significant differences between the groups with regard to the GMDS general quotient or other subscales, apart from the Personal/social subscale, where the HEU group performed significantly more poorly than the HUU participants (p=0.026). This difference is probably a result of cultural differences between the groups, as 76% of HEU and only 15% of HUU participants were of Xhosa origin.

Discussion. There was no difference in neurodevelopmental outcome at 18 months between the HEU and HUU groups.

S Afr J CH 2012;6(2):51-55.

Successful prevention of mother-to-child transmission (PMTCT) programmes have resulted in a decrease in vertical transmission of HIV to below 5%.1 In South Africa, with an antenatal HIV prevalence rate of around 30%, about a quarter of infants born are therefore classified as HIV exposed but uninfected (HEU). HIV affects children both directly and indirectly, and there is evidence of increased infectious mortality and morbidity in the HEU infant.2 There remains uncertainty regarding possible mechanisms of increased susceptibility and also the preventive measures to reduce these effects. Potential factors include increased exposure to infections and immune abnormalities in the infant, socio-economic difficulties, poor maternal health (including mental health), lack of parental care, reduced breastfeeding and unsuitable feeding practices.3 Conflicting data have been published about toxicity to the fetus of antiretroviral drugs (ARVs) and their effects on neurodevelopment.4 All these factors could potentially affect child development.

Of the published research on HEU children in Africa, only a few studies have high methodological quality using control groups and systematic validated measures of cognitive function.7 Msellati et al. in Rwanda demonstrated no difference in neurodevelopmental outcome between HEU and HIV-unexposed children (HUU), i.e. infants born to HIV-uninfected mothers.8 A Ugandan study also showed no significant difference between a group of HEU and HUU infants.9 , 10 On the other hand, Boivin et al. demonstrated deficits in cognitive performance in HEU children in Zaire compared with HUU controls,11 while Van Rie et al. found that HEU preschool children in the Democratic Republic of the Congo had poorer motor development and expressive language than HIV-unexposed controls.12 The authors argued that socio-economic differences between these groups rather than the inherent consequences of in utero HIV exposure may have accounted for the poorer outcome.

In South Africa the availability of ARVs and PMTCT, while decreasing the number of HIV-infected infants, has significantly increased the number of HEU infants born to HIV-infected mothers.1 It is important to determine whether any neurodevelopmental differences exist between HEU and HUU children, in order to facilitate the development and implementation of appropriate interventions for this growing population of infants.

Aims and objectives

The primary objective was to compare the neurodevelopmental outcome of infants who were exposed to HIV in utero but were uninfected (HEU) with that of infants born to HIV-uninfected mothers (HUU). A secondary objective was to identify markers for poor neurodevelopmental outcome in either group.

Methods

Recruitment

Participants were recruited from the postnatal maternity wards of Tygerberg Hospital, Western Cape, for a pilot study of the innate immune abnormalities in HEU infants. Tygerberg Hospital is one of two tertiary academic hospitals servicing the city and surrounds of Cape Town, Western Cape province, South Africa. It serves as the teaching hospital for Stellenbosch University. Patients accessing care are generally from lower socio-economic communities and are predominantly Afrikaans- or Xhosa-speaking. Participants were recruited consecutively over a 16-week period from March to June 2009. Mothers’ HIV infection status was confirmed on presentation in labour using standard HIV testing algorithms.13

The study protocol was approved by the Human Research Ethics Committee, Faculty of Health Sciences, Stellenbosch University (N08/10/289).

Inclusion and exclusion criteria

All infants who tested HIV negative (HIV-DNA-PCR) at 2, 6 and 12 weeks of age were included. Participants had to be between 17 and 19 months of age and physically healthy on the day of neurodevelopmental assessment. HIV-infected infants were excluded from the study.

Data collection

Information regarding pregnancy, birth history, weight gain, previous illnesses and hospitalisation, maternal characteristics and family history was obtained from the caregiver, hospital medical records and the child’s Road-to-Health card (immunisation record). Head circumference, weight and length were plotted on charts from the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (USA), and a developmental and neurological examination was performed.

Instruments

The Griffiths Mental Developmental Scales (GMDS) 0 - 2 years14 were administered. There are 5 subscales: Locomotor, Personal/social, Hearing and speech, Eye and hand co-ordination, and Performance. The GMDS have been adapted for South African children. Standard instructions and questions are available in English, Afrikaans and Xhosa. Although this tool has been widely used in South Africa,15 it has yet to be validated and standardised on South African children. The mother or a primary caregiver was present during the assessment, which was carried out in the child’s home language by one of two developmental paediatricians. An interpreter, also a trained GMDS administrator, helped the paediatricians to complete the scales with Xhosa-speaking patients. The paediatricians initially assessed a participant together and reached consensus on discrepant pass or fail test items, until it was felt that scoring was of a similar standard. The testers were blinded to the child’s HIV exposure status.

Data analysis

Statistica (Release version) 10 (Statsoft, Inc Tulsa, OK, USA) was used for analysis. Categorical data were analysed using either Fisher’s two-tailed analysis or Pearson’s chi-square analysis. The differences in numerical data (i.e. gestational age, birth weight, maternal age, weight, length, head circumference, chronological age and hospitalisation) among the two groups were analysed using t-tests. A Mann-Whitney U-test was used to determine differences in the GMDS scores.

Results

Twenty-five HIV-infected and 28 HIV-uninfected mothers were recruited from the postnatal wards at Tygerberg Hospital and their infants (27 HEU and 28 HUU) were enrolled 2 weeks after delivery. Of the original 55 infants enrolled, 39 presented for the visit at 18 months. Fifteen infants (6 HUU and 9 HEU) were lost to follow-up and 1 infant (HUU) died before 12 months. Thirty-seven of the remaining 39 infants underwent neurodevelopmental assessment, as one parent declined consent and one missed the appointment.

Among the 37 participants, there were 17 children who were HEU and 20 who were HUU. There was no significant difference between the groups with regard to gender, gestation, birth weight, mode of delivery or maternal age and education (Table 1). However, the HUU participants were predominantly Afrikaans language speakers (85%) while the HEU participants were predominantly Xhosa (76%).

There was no significant difference between HEU and HUU participants with regard to the general quotient and 4 of the 5 GMDS subscales, although both HEU and HUU group means were lower than the standardised mean (Table 2). However, the HEU group performed significantly more poorly than the HUU participants on the Personal/social subscale of the GMDS (Fig. 1). Specific items on the Personal/social scale which differentiated the groups included:

• Item 42: ‘shows shoes on request’ – correctly indicated by 85% of the HUU but only 47% of HEU participants

• Item 43: ‘mother reports that child uses spoon himself but spills some’ – passed by 85% of the HUU versus 59% of HEU participants

• Item 45: ‘shows one part of a doll’s body on request, e.g. hands, hair, feet, eyes, nose’ – passed by 65% of HUU versus 41% of HEU participants.

Five of the HEU and 2 of the HUU infants required at least one hospital admission for acute infection during their first 18 months of life, while 3 of the 5 HEU infants had two hospital admissions (Table 3). However there was no statistically significant difference between the groups with regard to the number of infants with one or more hospitalisations (p=0.21). An unexpected finding was the anthropometric differences between the groups. More children in the HUU group were stunted (Fig. 2). Two HUU participants had height-for-age z-scores (HAZ) scores more than 2 standard deviations (SD) below the mean, and 5 HUU participants had HAZ scores that fell between 1 and 2 SD below the mean. Only 1 HEU participant had a HAZ score between 1 and 2 SD below the mean, and none was greater than 2 SD. There were no infants in either group with weight-for-length z-scores (WLZ) more than 2 SD below the mean at 18 months. Although there was no statistically significant difference in head circumference between groups (0.21), 3 of the HEU infants had head circumferences above the 97th centile and 3 of the HUU infants had head circumferences below the 3rd centile.

The groups differed significantly in feeding patterns. All but 1 of the HEU infants were formula-fed, while all HUU infants were breastfed for a median of 12 weeks.

A significantly greater percentage of mothers in the HUU group (45%) versus the HEU group (11%) admitted to smoking during pregnancy (p=0.036).

Discussion

There was no significant difference in the General quotients of HEU and HUU participants, which correlates with findings of previous studies in Africa.8 However, the Personal/social subquotient was significantly lower in the HEU group. It is difficult to ascertain whether the lower mean Personal/social score was due to the biological and environmental exposures associated with having an HIV-positive mother or to a confounding effect of cultural differences between the groups which manifested as different child-rearing practices. For example, items in the GMDS such as spoon feeding and exposure to dolls are not culturally universal activities. The HUU participants were predominantly Afrikaans speaking, while the HEU participants were largely Xhosa speaking. When Personal/social subquotients were grouped primarily by home language and not by HIV exposure, the difference in scores between the Afrikaans and Xhosa groups was even more significant (p=0.015). The influence of cultural differences has been supported by Cockcroft et al., who found that black South African infants aged between 13 and 16 months performed significantly more poorly on the Personal/social scale compared with a British sample.15 Luiz also postulated that this subscale was the most culturally biased.18

Infant nutrition, specifically stunting, may also adversely affect development. Increased stunting among the HUU participants was an unexpected finding. Stunting has been found to be associated with poor neurodevelopmental outcome.19 It could be postulated that stunting was a confounder that adversely affected the HUU developmental scores, thus minimising the difference between the HUU and HEU groups. A significantly greater percentage of mothers in the HUU group admitted to smoking during pregnancy, and 3 of the mothers, all HIV uninfected, admitted to drinking alcohol. The HUU group may therefore have had other risk factors that could potentially have lowered their developmental scores.

All but 1 of the HEU participants had been exposed to ARVs, either as a result of PMTCT prophylaxis or combination ARV therapy given to their mothers, but no neurological abnormalities attributable to ARVs were evident at 18 months.

The strengths of the GMDS are that the tool has been extensively used on this age group in South Africa with Xhosa and Afrikaans translations, and it does appear to be reliable in picking up differences between groups.15

Limitations

Limitations of the study include the number of children lost to follow-up from the original cohort. By the 18-month time point when development was assessed, a large number of infants had been lost from the study as a result of relocating or mothers returning to work. Secondly, the small sample size precluded multivariable analysis to adjust for the effects of confounding factors. In particular this meant that any effect of the cultural imbalance between the HEU and HUU groups could not be adequately adjusted for. Finally, the effects of ARVs on neurodevelopmental outcomes could not be ascertained as all but 1 of the HEU children had been exposed to ARVs at some point in time.

Conclusion

There was no difference in performance on the GMDS between HEU and HUU infants, except for the Personal/social scale, where the HEU participants did significantly worse. This was probably accounted for by cultural variations between the groups.

Recommendations

Studies with a larger sample size attempting to control for confounding factors through matching or adjustment are recommended. Evaluating children’s development at 12 months of age may reduce attrition. It would be beneficial to review items on the Personal/social subscale for cultural bias.

Acknowledgements. This work was supported in part by National Institute of Allergy and Infectious Diseases, NIH, Grant number: N01 AI50023, the Martha Piper Fund, and the Peter Wall Institute for Advanced Studies. ME was supported in part through the National Health Laboratory Service Research Trust (grant number TY94171), the National Health Laboratory Service (K Fund numbers KNC97 and KNC103), the Poliomyelitis Research Foundation (grant numbers 10/02, 10/31 and 11/37), and the Harry Crossley Foundation, 2009 - 2011 (grant numbers 5415 and 5762). MT is supported by grants from the National Institute of Alcoholism and Alcohol Abuse (USA), the National Institute on Drug Abuse (USA), and the National Research Foundation (South Africa). The authors would like to thank Staff Nurse S Sylvester and Ms N R Khethelo for their practical and logistical contributions.

Author contributions. PS contributed to protocol design, carried out the interviews and neurodevelopmental assessments, contributed to the understanding of the results and drafted the initial article as well as revisions. BL contributed to the conceptualisation of the project, protocol design, carried out the interviews and neurodevelopmental assessments, contributed to the understanding of the results and assisted in the write-up and revision of the article. MT contributed to the understanding of the results and assisted in the write-up and revision of the article. JH provided data analysis support. ME was the supervisor, contributed to the conceptualisation of the project and assisted in the revision of the article. We gratefully acknowledge the data provided by co-workers Amy Slogrove, Shalena Naidoo, Kevin Ho and Gareth Mercer.

References
1. Goga A, Dinh T-H, Dlamini N, et al. Impact of the national prevention of mother to child transmission (PMTCT) program on mother-to-child transmission of HIV (MTCT), South Africa, 2010. Presented at the 6th International AIDS Society Conference, Rome, 17-20 July 2011. Abstract MOAC0206.

1. Goga A, Dinh T-H, Dlamini N, et al. Impact of the national prevention of mother to child transmission (PMTCT) program on mother-to-child transmission of HIV (MTCT), South Africa, 2010. Presented at the 6th International AIDS Society Conference, Rome, 17-20 July 2011. Abstract MOAC0206.

2. Slogrove AL, Cotton MF, Esser MM. Severe infections in HIV-exposed uninfected infants: Clinical evidence of immunodeficiency. J Trop Pediatr 2010;56(2):75-81.

2. Slogrove AL, Cotton MF, Esser MM. Severe infections in HIV-exposed uninfected infants: Clinical evidence of immunodeficiency. J Trop Pediatr 2010;56(2):75-81.

3. Filteau S. The HIV-exposed, uninfected African child. Trop Med Int Health 2009;14(3):276-287.

3. Filteau S. The HIV-exposed, uninfected African child. Trop Med Int Health 2009;14(3):276-287.

4. Heidari S, Mofenson L, Cotton MF, Marlink R, Cahn P, Katabira E. Antiretroviral drugs for preventing mother-to-child transmission of HIV: A review of potential effects on HIV-exposed but uninfected children. Acquir Immune Defic Syndr 2011;57:290-296.

4. Heidari S, Mofenson L, Cotton MF, Marlink R, Cahn P, Katabira E. Antiretroviral drugs for preventing mother-to-child transmission of HIV: A review of potential effects on HIV-exposed but uninfected children. Acquir Immune Defic Syndr 2011;57:290-296.

5. Williams PL, Marino M, Malee K, Brogly S, Hughes MD, Mofenson LM. Neurodevelopment and in utero antiretroviral exposure of HIV-exposed uninfected infants. Pediatrics 2010;125(2);e250-260. Epub 2010 Jan18. http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/125/2/e250.full.pdf (accessed 9 January 2012).

5. Williams PL, Marino M, Malee K, Brogly S, Hughes MD, Mofenson LM. Neurodevelopment and in utero antiretroviral exposure of HIV-exposed uninfected infants. Pediatrics 2010;125(2);e250-260. Epub 2010 Jan18. http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/125/2/e250.full.pdf (accessed 9 January 2012).

6. Blanche S, Tardieu M, Rustin P, et al. Persistent mitochondrial dysfunction and perinatal exposure to antiretroviral nucleoside analogues. Lancet 1999;354:1084-1089.

6. Blanche S, Tardieu M, Rustin P, et al. Persistent mitochondrial dysfunction and perinatal exposure to antiretroviral nucleoside analogues. Lancet 1999;354:1084-1089.

7. Sherr L, Mueller J, Varrall R. A systematic review of cognitive development and child human immunodeficiency virus infection. Psychol Health Med 2009;14(4):387-404.

7. Sherr L, Mueller J, Varrall R. A systematic review of cognitive development and child human immunodeficiency virus infection. Psychol Health Med 2009;14(4):387-404.

8. Msellati P, Lepage P, Hitimana D, Van Goetham C, Van de Perre P, Dabis F. Neurodevelopmental testing of children born to human immunodeficiency virus type 1 seropositive and seronegative mothers: A prospective cohort study in Kigali, Rwanda. Pediatrics 1993;92(6):843-848.

8. Msellati P, Lepage P, Hitimana D, Van Goetham C, Van de Perre P, Dabis F. Neurodevelopmental testing of children born to human immunodeficiency virus type 1 seropositive and seronegative mothers: A prospective cohort study in Kigali, Rwanda. Pediatrics 1993;92(6):843-848.

9. Drotar D, Olness K, Wiznitzer M, et al. Neurodevelopmental outcomes of Ugandan infants with human immunodeficiency virus type 1 infection. Pediatrics 1997;100(1). http://www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/100/1/e5 (accessed 14 March 2011). http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.100.1.e5

9. Drotar D, Olness K, Wiznitzer M, et al. Neurodevelopmental outcomes of Ugandan infants with human immunodeficiency virus type 1 infection. Pediatrics 1997;100(1). http://www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/100/1/e5 (accessed 14 March 2011). http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.100.1.e5

10. Bagenda D, Nassali A, Kalyesubula I, et al. Health, neurologic, and cognitive status of HIV-infected, long-surviving, and antiretroviral-naïve Ugandan children. Pediatrics 2006;117(3):729-740.

10. Bagenda D, Nassali A, Kalyesubula I, et al. Health, neurologic, and cognitive status of HIV-infected, long-surviving, and antiretroviral-naïve Ugandan children. Pediatrics 2006;117(3):729-740.

11. Boivin M, Davies AG, Mokili JK, Green SD, Giordani B, Cutting WAM. A preliminary evaluation of the cognitive and motor effects of pediatric HIV infection in Zairian children. Health Psychol 1995;14(1):13-21.

11. Boivin M, Davies AG, Mokili JK, Green SD, Giordani B, Cutting WAM. A preliminary evaluation of the cognitive and motor effects of pediatric HIV infection in Zairian children. Health Psychol 1995;14(1):13-21.

12. Van Rie A, Mupuala A, Dow A. Impact of HIV/AIDS epidemic on the neurodevelopment of preschool-aged children in Kinshasha, Democratic Republic of the Congo. Pediatrics 2008;122(1):e123-128. http://www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/122/1/e123 (accessed 20 March 2011).

12. Van Rie A, Mupuala A, Dow A. Impact of HIV/AIDS epidemic on the neurodevelopment of preschool-aged children in Kinshasha, Democratic Republic of the Congo. Pediatrics 2008;122(1):e123-128. http://www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/122/1/e123 (accessed 20 March 2011).

13. National Department of Health, South Africa, and South African National AIDS Council. Clinical Guidelines: PMTCT (Prevention of Mother-to-Child Transmission). http://www.fidssa.co.za/images/PMTCT_Guidelines.pdf (accessed 2 May 2012).

13. National Department of Health, South Africa, and South African National AIDS Council. Clinical Guidelines: PMTCT (Prevention of Mother-to-Child Transmission). http://www.fidssa.co.za/images/PMTCT_Guidelines.pdf (accessed 2 May 2012).

14. Griffiths R. The Griffiths Mental Development Scales: From Birth to 2 Years: Manual. Rev. Huntley M. Oxford: The Test Agency, 1996.

14. Griffiths R. The Griffiths Mental Development Scales: From Birth to 2 Years: Manual. Rev. Huntley M. Oxford: The Test Agency, 1996.

15. Cockcroft K, Amod Z, Soellaart B. Level of maternal education and performance of black South African infants on the 1996 Griffiths Mental Development Scales. African Journal of Psychiatry 2008;11(1):44-50.

15. Cockcroft K, Amod Z, Soellaart B. Level of maternal education and performance of black South African infants on the 1996 Griffiths Mental Development Scales. African Journal of Psychiatry 2008;11(1):44-50.

16. Amod Z, Cockcroft K, Soellart B. Use of the 1996 Griffiths Mental Scales for infants: a pilot study with a black South African sample. Journal of Child and Adolescent Mental Health 2007;19(2):123-130.

16. Amod Z, Cockcroft K, Soellart B. Use of the 1996 Griffiths Mental Scales for infants: a pilot study with a black South African sample. Journal of Child and Adolescent Mental Health 2007;19(2):123-130.

17. Perez EM, Hendricks MK, Beard JL, et al. Mother-infant interactions and infant development are altered by maternal iron deficiency anemia. J Nutr 2005;135(4):850-855.

17. Perez EM, Hendricks MK, Beard JL, et al. Mother-infant interactions and infant development are altered by maternal iron deficiency anemia. J Nutr 2005;135(4):850-855.

18. Luiz DM, Foxcroft CD, Stewart R. The construct validity of the Griffiths Scales of Mental Development. Child Care Health Dev 2001;27(1):73-83.

18. Luiz DM, Foxcroft CD, Stewart R. The construct validity of the Griffiths Scales of Mental Development. Child Care Health Dev 2001;27(1):73-83.

19. Walker SP, Wachs TD, Gardner JM, et al. Child development: risk factors for adverse outcomes in developing countries. Lancet 2007;369:145-157.

19. Walker SP, Wachs TD, Gardner JM, et al. Child development: risk factors for adverse outcomes in developing countries. Lancet 2007;369:145-157.

Fig. 1. Mean general quotients and subquotients obtained on the Griffiths Mental Development Scales: comparison of HIV-exposed but uninfected (HEU) and HIV-unexposed and uninfected (HUU) infants.

Fig. 2. Comparison of length-for-age z-scores between HIV-unexposed uninfected (HUU) and HIV-exposed but uninfected (HEU) infants.

Table 1. Maternal and infant characteristics: comparison of HIV-exposed but uninfected (HEU) with HIV-unexposed
uninfected (HUU) infants

Demographics

HEU (n=17)

HUU (n=20)

p-value

Gender, male (n (%))

6 (35)

10 (50)

0.093

Gestation (wks) (n)

<37

≥37

4

13

4

16

0.65

Birth weight (g) (median (range))

2 980

(1 900 - 3 820)

3 068

(2 080 - 3 600)

0.78

Mode of delivery (n (%))

Vertex delivery

Breech delivery

Caesarean section

16 (94)

0

1 (6)

19 (95)

1 (5)

0

0.362

Maternal age at delivery (yrs) (median (range))

27

(19 - 41)

28

(19 - 44)

0.51

Maternal education (final grade attained in formal schooling) (n (%))

≤7

8 - 10

>10

3 (17)

8 (47)

6 (35)

3 (15)

11 (55)

6 (30)

0.745

Language (n (%))

Afrikaans

Xhosa

Mixed Xhosa/Eng/French

4 (23)

11 (64)

2 (11)

17 (85)

3 (15)

0.026

Table 2. Mean quotients and subquotients on the Griffiths Mental Development Scales: comparison of HIV-exposed but uninfected (HEU) with HIV-unexposed (HUU) infants

Griffiths quotients

Group

Mean

Median

Minimum

Maximum

SD

p-value

General quotient

HEU

HUU

83.05

87.45

84

89

60

68

97

98

11.31

8.65

0.190

Locomotor

HEU

HUU

82.76

86.75

83

84

56

62

107

112

16.33

14.25

0.433

Personal/social

HEU

HUU

79.35

88.45

81

89

59

68

102

111

11.20

11.41

0.026

Hearing and language

HEU

HUU

89.11

89.25

91

89

72

64

102

107

9.37

10.70

0.968

Eye-hand

co-ordination

HEU

HUU

89.52

93.25

96

92

60

75

118

118

18.16

11.26

0.451

Performance

HEU

HUU

76

83.4

82

84

50

61

102

106

13.71

16.01

0.139

Table 3. Possible confounders: comparison of HIV-exposed but uninfected (HEU) with HIV-unexposed (HUU) infants

Variable

HEU (n=17)

HUU (n=20)

p-value

Low birth weight

(<2 500 g) (n (%))

2 (11)

1 (5)

Head circumference (cm) (mean)

46.92

Macrocephaly (n=3)

46.09 cm

Microcephaly (n=2)

0.21

Weight for age

(mean z-scores)

0.2773

-0.3985

0.016

Length for age

(mean z-scores)

0.2272

-0.6266

0.045

Weight for length

(mean z-scores)

0.6105

-0.1419

0.040

Physical and neurological findings

No major neurological or physical

abnormalities

Achilles tendon contractures (n=1)

Fetal alcohol spectrum disorder (n=1)

Strabismus (n=1)

Serous otitis media (n=1)

SD = standard deviation.



Article Views

Abstract views: 4630
Full text views: 6665

Comments on this article

*Read our policy for posting comments here